A Comparison of Seventh Grade Students’ Brian Wave Patterns During Arithmetic Testing

Authors

  • Kanok Panthong College of Research Methodology and Cognitive Science, Burapha University
  • Piyathip Pradujprom College of Research Methodology and Cognitive Science, Burapha University

Keywords:

gender, students’ ability, event-related potentials, arithmetic testing task

Abstract

The purposes of this study were to; 1) compare the brainwave patterns of students while performing arithmetic tasks for measuring three cognitive processes (remembering, understanding, and applying), categorized by gender and students’ ability, and 2) to investigate an interaction effect between gender and students’ ability on brainwave patterns of students while performing aforementioned tasks. The sample group consisted 40 grade-seven students of Preechanusas School in Chonburi (students took a mathematics subject at 6th grade) who volunteered to participate in the study. The participants were divided into two groups; high and low ability. The research instruments were; 1) a general information questionnaire (gender, health condition, and ability to see), 2) Oldfield’s evaluation form in evaluating the right-hand limit, 3) the arithmetic test for measuring the 3 cognitive processes, 4) the arithmetic testing task for measuring the 3 cognitive processes via computer with Stim 2 program, 5) SynAmps RT, and 64-channel Quik-Cap, and 6) a computer with Intel core i5-2400 (3.1 GHz) and Microsoft Windows XP Professional operating system. Data were analyzed using descriptive statistics and two-way multivariate analysis of variance (Two-Way MANOVA). The results showed that amplitude and latency of P300 of the participants, classified by ability and gender of the participants, while performing arithmetic tasks for measuring the 3cognitive processes, were significantly difference at the .05 level at the electrode sites of FT8 F2 F6 FC3 FC1 FCZ FC2 FC4 CP2 CP4 CP6 C5 C3 C1 CZ C2 C6 TP7 TP8 T7 T8 CP6 P5 P3 P1 PZ P2 P4 P6 P8 PO7 PO5 PO3 POZ PO4 PO6 PO8 OZ and O2. When focusing on the participants’ ability, it was found that for remembering-based arithmetic task, the students with high ability had higher P300 amplitude than students with low ability. However, at some electrode sites, the students with low ability had higher P300 amplitude than the students with high ability for remembering-and understanding-based tasks. Furthermore, for applying-based arithmetic task, the students with high ability had longer P300 latency than students with low ability. When focusing on the participants’ gender, it was found that for remembering and applying-based arithmetic task, female students had higher and longer P300 amplitude and latency than male students. However, at some electrode sites, the male students had higher P300 amplitude than the female students for understanding-based tasks. In addition, female students had longer P300 latency than male students for all arithmetic tasks. Furthermore, a two-way MANOVA demonstrated the significant interaction effect between gender and students’ ability on P300 amplitude and latency for remembering and understanding-arithmetic tasks at p<.05 at FC4, FC6, F2, F6, C3, C1, C4, C6, P1, and P7 electrode sites.

Downloads

References

Anderson, L. W., Krathwohl, D. R., Airasian, P., Cruikshank, K., Mayer, R., Pintrich, P. R., … Wittrock, M. C. (2001). A Taxonomy for Learning, Teaching and Assessing: A Revision of Bloom’s Taxonomy. New York : Longman Publishing.

Artz, AF, & Armour-Thomas, E. (1992). Development of a cognitive-metacognitive framework for protocol analysis of mathematical problem solving in small groups. Cognition and Instruction. 9 (2), pp. 137-175.

Bajric, J., Rösler, F., Heil, M., & Hennighausen, E. (1999). On separating processes of event categorization, task preparation, and mental rotation proper in a handedness recognition task. Journal Psychophysiol. 36 (3), pp 399–408.

Benbow, C. P., & Stanley, J. C. (1980). Sex differences in mathematical ability: fact or artifact?. Science. 210 (4475), pp. 1262-1264.

Chittapalo, O. (1998). General Psychology. Bangkok: Chitwat Printing House. (in Thai)

Davis, N., Cannistraci, C. J., Rogers, B. P., Gatenby J. C., Fuchs L. S., Anderson A. W., & Gore, J. C. (2009). The neural correlates of calculation ability in children: an fMRI study. Magnetic Resonance Imaging. 27 (9), pp 1187–1197.

Dehaene, S., Molko, N., Cohen, L., & Wilson, A. J. (2004). Arithmetic and the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 14 (2), pp. 218–224.

Dickhauser, O., & Meyer, W. (2006). Gender differences in young children's math ability attributions. Psychology Science. 48 (1), pp. 3-16.

Donchin, E., & Coles, M. G. (1988). Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?. Behavioral and brain sciences. 11 (3), pp. 357-374.

Ebmeier, K. P., Steele, J. D., MacKenzie, D. M., O'Carroll, R. E., Kydd, R. R., Glabus, M. F., … Goodwin, G. M. (1995). Cognitive brain potentials and regional cerebral blood flow equivalents during two-and three-sound auditory “oddball tasks”. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. 95 (6), pp. 434-443.

Emmerson, R. Y., Dustman, R. E., Shearer, D. E., & Turner, C. W. (1989). P3 latency and symbol digit performance correlations in aging. Experimental aging research. 15 (3), pp. 151-159.

Fabiani, M., Gratton, G. and Federmeier, K.D. (2007). Event-related brain potentials : Methods, theory, and applications. in Handbook of Psychophysiology. 3rd ed. Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.

Gallagher, A. M., & De Lisi, R. (1994). Gender differences in Scholastic Aptitude Test: Mathematics problem solving among high-ability students. Journal of Educational Psychology. 86 (2), pp. 204-211.

Gallagher, A., Levin, J., & Cahalan, C. (2002). Cognitive patterns of gender differences on mathematics admissions tests. ETS Research Report Series. 2002 (2), pp. i-30.

Ganley, C., & Lubienski, S. (2016). Current Research on Gender Differences in Math, National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Retrieved December 5, 2016, from http://www.nctm.org /Publications/Teaching-Children-Mathematics/Blog/Current-Research-on-Gender-differences -in-Math/

Ganley, C. M. & Vasilyeva, M. (2011). Sex differences in the relation between math performance, spatial skills, and attitudes. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 32 (4), pp. 235-242.

Guiso, L., Monte, F., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Culture, gender, and math. Science-New york then Washington. 320 (5880), pp. 1164-1165.

Handy, C. T. (2005). Event-Related Potentials: A Methods Handbook. Cambridge, Mass, USA : MIT Press.

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science. 269 (5220), pp. 41-45.

Houlihan, M., Stelmack, R., & Campbell, K. (1998). Intelligence and the effects of perceptual processing demands, task difficulty and processing speed on P300, reaction time and movement time. Intelligence. 26 (1), pp. 9-25.

Iguchi, Y., & Hashimoto, I. (2000). Sequential information processing during a mental arithmetic is reflected in the time course of event-related brain potentials. Clinical Neurophysiology. 111 (2), pp. 204-213.

Johnson, R. (1986). A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. Psychophysiology. 23, pp. 367–384.

Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1994). Mental models and probabilistic thinking. Cognition. 50 (1-3), pp. 189-209.

Kennedy, L., & Tipps, S. (1994). Guiding Children's Learning of Mathematics. Belmont, CA : Wadsworth.

Kirino, E., Belger, A., Goldman-Rakic, P., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Prefrontal activation evoked by infrequent target and novel stimuli in a visual target detection task: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience. 20 (17), pp. 6612-6618.

Kong, J., Wang, Y., Shang, H., Xiuzhen Yang, X., & Zhuang, D. (1999). Brain potentials during mental arithmetic-effects of problem difficulty on event-related brain potentials. Neuroscience Letters. 260 (3), pp. 169–172.

Kotchoubey, B. (2006). Event-related potentials, cognition, and behavior: A biological approach. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 30 (1), pp. 42–65.

Krathwohl, D. R. (2002). A revision of Bloom's taxonomy: An overview. Theory into practice. 41 (4), pp. 212-218.

McMillan, J. H., & Schumacher, S. (2010). Research in education: Evidence Based Inquiry. 7th ed. New York : Longman.

Mertens, R., & Polich, J. (1997). P300 from a single-stimulus paradigm: passive versus active tasks and stimulus modality. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/EvokedPotentialsSection. 104 (6), pp. 488-497.

Ministry of Education. (2008). The Basic Education Core Curriculum B.E. 2551 (A.D. 2008) . Retrieved June 5, 2015, from http://www.curriculum51.net/upload/cur-51.pdf (in Thai)

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics Commission on Standards for School Mathematics. (1989). Curriculum and evaluation standards for school mathematics. Natl Council of Teachers of.

National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2017). NCTM 2017 Annual Meeting and Exposition. Retrieved August 28, 2017, from http://www.nctm.org/News-and-Calendar/Calendar-Events/NCTM/NCTM-2017-Annual-Meeting-and-Exposition/)

Núñez-Peña, M. I., Gracia-Bafalluy, M., & Tubau, E. (2011). Individual differences in arithmetic skill reflected in event-related brain potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 80 (2), pp. 143-149.

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 9 (1), pp. 97-113.

Panthong, K., Chadcham, S. and Pitukvattananon, K. (2011). Effects of Item Diffi culty and Students’ Ability on EEG: An Event-Related Potentials Study During Arithmetic Testing. Research Methodology and Cognitive Science. 9 (2), pp. 62-77. (in Thai)

Pauli, P., Lutzenberger, W., Birbaumer, N., Rickard, T.C., & Bourne Jr. L. E. (1996). Neurophysiological correlates of mental arithmetic. Psychophysiology. 33 (5), pp. 522 –529.

Pelosi, L., Holly, M., Slade, T., Hayward, M., Barrett, G., & Blumhardt, L. D. (1992). Event-related potential (ERP) correlates of performance of intelligence tests.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section. 84 (6), pp. 515-520.

Polich, J., Alexander, J. E., Bauer, L. O., Kuperman, S., Morzorati, S., O'Connor, S. J., … Begleiter, H. (1997). P300 topography of amplitude/latency correlations. Brain topography. 9 (4), pp. 275-282.

Taylor, P.C.J., & Thut, G. (2012). Brian activity underlying visual perception and attention as inferred from TMS-EEG: A Review. Brain Stimulation. 5 (2), pp. 124-129.

Vaquero, E., Cardoso, M. J., Vazque, M., & Gomez, C. M. (2004). Gender differences in event-related potentials during visual-spatial attention. International journal of neuroscience. 114 (4), pp. 541-557.

Weisberg, R. W., & Reeves, L. M. (2013). Cognition: From Memory to Creativity. John Wiley & Sons.

Wilson, G. F., Swain, C., & Ullsperger, P. (1998). ERP components elicited in response to warning stimuli: the influence of task diffculty. Bio Psychol.. 47 (2), pp. 137–158.

Zamarian, L., Ischebeck A., & Delazer, M. (2009). Neuroscience of learning arithmetic–Evidence from brain imaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 33 (6), pp. 909 - 925.

Downloads

Published

2019-05-01

How to Cite

Panthong, K. ., & Pradujprom, P. . (2019). A Comparison of Seventh Grade Students’ Brian Wave Patterns During Arithmetic Testing. Dhonburi Rajabhat University Journal, 13(1), 34–50. retrieved from https://so02.tci-thaijo.org/index.php/journaldru/article/view/253721

Issue

Section

Reseach Articles